|
Everyone who speaks in public knows that hand gestures just go with the territory. Effective public address requires that one gesture in appropriate ways and at appropriate times in the course of speaking. Preachers know that very well. Preachers or not, we are also aware that gesturing with the hands is an essential part of the very fabric of conversational speech. Who among us could actually keep our hands still during the course of an animated conversation? To talk is to gesture. In fact, what is odd, and often disconcerting, is when someone talks without gesturing. There is invariably something cold about that; something seems wrong, incomplete, even untrustworthy when that happens. What do gestures do? What are they for? Why do we use them? What speech teachers since Aristotle and Cicero have taught us is that gestures are an integral part of the communication process itself. Gestures help us communicate. They enable us to give emphasis to particular words and phases. They assist us in explaining things, since we can "draw," as it were, in the air. Gestures can also supplement, often very powerfully, the emotions that we express verbally, since our hands can also convey anger, or joy, or frustration, or any number of other deep-seated feelings. Gestures are for communicating. We have known that for a long time. The speaker who does not master the art of gesturing is something less of a speaker than he or she could be. Over the last few years, however, several new lines of research have been undertaken with regard to gestures in communication--research that has particular relevance for public speakers--among them preachers. Two of these sets of research findings have recently been summarized in scientific journals. In both cases, the research suggests that gestures are not JUST for communication after all. Other important functions of gesturing are being uncovered. The first is what appears to be a direct link between gestures and memory. While we have little idea yet about how it works, or even how well it works, the evidence is beginning to grow that our powers of recall are significantly improved . . . if, in our effort to recall something we have learned, we gesture with our hands in the process of talking with ourselves or others. An important study of this was reported in a recent issue of the American Journal of Psychology. Two researchers, working with several volunteer groups, asked the subjects vocabulary questions; i.e., the meanings of words that all would be expected to know. One group was allowed to think (and converse with the questioner) with hands free and normal, gesturing as always in the speaking interchange. However, members of the second group were asked the same questions and allowed to think and converse--but without using their hands. They were required to speak while holding onto a railing in front of them with both hands. Subjects for asked, for example, to name "an ancient instrument used for calculations." Consistently, to the researchers' surprise, the subjects whose hands were "hooked" to the railing were significantly less likely to come up with the word "abacus" than those whose hands were free to gesture during the "thought" process. As the scientists would say, what we have here is a hypothesis; but a remarkably interesting and promising one: that gesturing freely is an aid to remembering while one is speaking. The second study, in many ways, is even more significant and basic. It was reported not long ago in the scientific journal, Nature. This time the researchers wanted to know if gesturing while speaking is a learned trait, something acquired in the process of watching others speak; or whether gesturing is an instinctual part of human speech and thought. To answer the question, the researchers worked with children and teenagers aged 8 to 18. The unique thing about the study was that half of the subjects were sighted, while the other half had been blind since birth--meaning that it was impossible for them to have seen anyone gesture while speaking. In several tests, the researchers asked each young subject to explain certain concepts. For example, each was asked to explain in any way he or she wished the relationship between an amount of water placed in a glass and a bowl. What the researchers found consistently was that the blind children--those who had never seen others gesture before--gestured just as much as the sighted children as they explained whether the glass or the bowl held more water. Moreover, the researchers discovered that blind children still used vigorous hand gestures even when the people they were talking to were also blind. The bottom line is that the evidence is growing that gesturing and thinking are very closely interwoven with each other. Gesturing is not a learned activity. It appears, rather, to be integrally connected to the processes of reasoning, thinking and problem-solving. Such mental activities, moreover, seem to be significantly enhanced by gesturing. It is true that there are people who, for various reasons, do not have the use of their hands but still manage to think and communicate well. But we are learning that gestures are about more than communication; they are also a part of the mind's very activity in remembering and speaking. We think better, we remember better, and we speak better when we gesture vigorously and well. What does this mean for you, the preacher? Several things, briefly. First, it means that, as a professional public speaker, you need to give attention to improving your gesturing as you speak. Give free rein to your gesturing--spontaneously, naturally, and as though you are engaged in a vigorous across-the-table conversation. In fact, the next time you are in such an uninhibited conversation, be keenly aware of the lively gesturing that is going on; and bring that kind of gesturing to your pulpit. Second, for preachers who read sermons from manuscripts, remembering what to say is not an issue--which may, in small part, account for the difficulty most manuscript preachers have in gesturing well. However, for preachers who work with--or without--notes in the pulpit, working on good gesturing is a way of working at remembering what one wants to say. That is not something that we have known before. Undoubtedly, in the future, we will learn a great deal more about how this works. But knowing the connection for now is of great value. Finally, it is remarkable to realize that clear thinking and good gesturing in the pulpit may really be part of each other. Up to now, gesturing has been considered an "add on" in public address. You gesture so that people will get the message better--a communication device that one "attaches" to what is being said. It appears that it may be much, much more than that. We gesture to think well. We gesture to remember better what we want to say. We gesture to speak more clearly. Good gesturing is every bit as much an aid to the speaker, the preacher, as it is to the listener! --Joseph M. Webb |
Gestures in Preaching: New Research Discoveries |